How many types of ganglion cells exist? The number of putative ga

How many types of ganglion cells exist? The number of putative ganglion cell types estimated in a series of five recent studies in the mouse was 11, 12, 14, 19, and 22 (review,

Masland, 2012). New cell types have emerged since those studies were conducted. The apparent number of ganglion cell types depends a lot on how they are counted: should ON and OFF variants of the same response see more pattern be considered as one cell type or two? Do the four cardinal direction preferences of DS cells represent four cell types or one? No matter how one counts, the number of types is surely not less than a dozen in any mammal yet studied, and many workers feel that the minimal number of structurally distinct types in the mouse, rabbit, cat, or monkey is in the neighborhood of 20. What can be

the uses of 20 types of ganglion cells? There is more extensive information for the rabbit retina Compound Library supplier than any other. The ganglion cell types for which a morphological/physiological identification is secure are as follows: a local edge detector, much like the “bug detector” described long ago in the frog by Maturana et al. (1960); ON-tonic and OFF-tonic cells; blue-ON and blue-OFF ganglion cells; an ON direction selective cell, which projects to the accessory optic system and subserves optokinetic nystagmus; an ON-OFF directionally selective cell, function unknown; two large, ON-transient or OFF-transient cells; a recently identified “transient ON-OFF ganglion cell,” which responds much like an ON-OFF DS cell but is not directionally selective and has a different stratification; a uniformity detector, which responds to changes in the visual input by decreasing its firing rate; cells selective to each of two preferred orientations; and the sparse intrinsically photosensitive (melanopsin) cells, whose long-lasting responses to light synchronize the circadian

oscillator, drive pupillary responses, and carry out other functions still being explored. In the mouse, a curiously shaped cell with a weak form of direction selectivity has been discovered, as has an apparent homolog of the local edge detector (Amthor et al., 1989; Ecker et al., 2010; Kim et al., most 2008; Levick, 1967; Rockhill et al., 2002; Roska and Werblin, 2001; Schmidt et al., 2011; Sivyer et al., 2010, 2011; Taylor and Smith, 2011; van Wyk et al., 2006, 2009; Vaney et al., 2012; Venkataramani and Taylor, 2010; Zhang et al., 2012). This may seem like a long list. Note, however, that there are nine modality-specific channels for touch, five for taste, and >300 for smell. Truly remarkable would have been for vision, said to occupy ∼50% of the cortex in primates (Van Essen, 2004), to have only the two types of retinal ganglion cell stressed in the standard canon.

These principles have long been recognized

These principles have long been recognized Olaparib cell line by Ethical Review Committees and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees as pivotal to their consideration of research protocols. As stated above, they are highlighted in the Guide as well as the EU Directive, both of which act as key references

for animal care and use worldwide. But the manner and extent of implementation of the 3Rs in animal-based research generally, and neuroscience in particular, vary considerably, and not all neuroscience investigators regard the 3Rs as either helpful or binding, much less as appropriate standards to be applied internationally. Since the ever-increasing globalization of scientific inquiry is leading to both greater collaboration and greater competition among scientists LY2835219 mw world-wide, a clear, consistent and balanced approach to the use of animals in research is becoming more necessary. Variation in practice in the use of animals makes collaboration,

the pooling of data and copublication of results more difficult. And it would surely be unacceptable if cutting ethical corners in the use of animals could give scientists an edge in competition with rival groups. Harmonization is called for, but the debate is whether this requires internationally mandated research standards and policies in relation to principles such as the 3Rs. Put simply, is there a need for neuroscientists internationally to promote implementation of the 3Rs? And would this limit or enhance the quality of their science? Some neuroscientists urge caution in considering harmonization of any standards applied to the use of animals in science. They argue that shared principles already exist across the scientific community and that, while there is value in identifying areas of agreement, a divergence of practice within a strong overall ethical framework is desirable. Proper evaluation and reporting on the outcome of such diverse practices could be the optimal route to best practice. While the goal of the 3Rs serves as a worthy

guideline for all neuroscientists, Thymidine kinase some researchers caution against embedding the 3Rs as a core value, perceiving a risk of raising false and unrealizable expectations or possibly jeopardizing important medical progress. On the other hand, some neuroscientists take the view that a key consideration for the animals used in their research is to minimize pain and distress and to improve well-being in accordance with their science. The 3Rs are seen by them as a powerful mnemonic for ethically appropriate behavior. This broad spectrum of views within the neuroscience community reflects the range of interpretations held by the authors of this opinion piece. However, we have become convinced of the value of sharing these views in a transparent manner and, through dialog, moving toward common ground.

Furthermore, contrast adaptation enhances information transmissio

Furthermore, contrast adaptation enhances information transmission at low contrast (Gaudry and Reinagel, 2007a). In the retina, a major goal is to understand how contrast adaptation arises in the circuitry at the level of synapses and intrinsic membrane properties. Contrast adaptation has been studied in several cell types of salamander retina, including cone photoreceptors and two of their postsynaptic targets: horizontal and bipolar cells. Neither cones nor horizontal www.selleckchem.com/products/Dasatinib.html cells adapt to contrast, and thus contrast adaptation

first appears beyond the point of cone glutamate release (Baccus and Meister, 2002 and Rieke, 2001). Bipolar cells, the excitatory interneurons that transmit cone signals to ganglion cells, do adapt to contrast (Baccus and Meister, 2002 and Rieke, 2001). The bipolar cell’s contrast adaptation is reflected in the excitatory membrane currents and membrane potential (Vm) of ganglion cells (salamander: Baccus and Meister, 2002 and Kim

and Rieke, 2001 and mammal: Beaudoin et al., 2007, Beaudoin et al., 2008, Manookin and Demb, 2006 and Zaghloul et al., 2005). However, this presynaptic mechanism for contrast adaptation explains only a portion of the adaptation in the ganglion cell’s firing rate (Kim and Rieke, Z-VAD-FMK 2001, Zaghloul et al., 2005, Manookin and Demb, 2006 and Beaudoin et al., 2007; 2008). Thus, the presynaptic mechanism combines with intrinsic mechanisms within the ganglion cell to reduce sensitivity during periods of high contrast. In dim light, where signaling depends on rods and rod bipolar cells, contrast adaptation depends predominantly Fossariinae on the ganglion cell’s intrinsic mechanism (Beaudoin et al., 2008). In theory, an intrinsic mechanism for contrast adaptation should sense changes in Vm during high-contrast exposure. During high contrast, a ganglion cell’s Vm spans a wide range and includes periods of both hyperpolarization (up to ∼10 mV) and depolarization (up to ∼20 mV) from

the resting potential (Vrest); the depolarizations are accompanied by increased firing. The durations of hyperpolarizations and depolarizations are determineds by the temporal filtering of retinal circuitry, which under light-adapted conditions shows band-pass tuning with peak sensitivity near ∼8 Hz; this tuning results in brief periods of depolarization and firing (∼50–100 msec) that are themselves separated by ∼100–200 msec (Berry et al., 1997, Zaghloul et al., 2005 and Beaudoin et al., 2007). Therefore, an intrinsic mechanism that suppresses firing at high contrast should recover with a time course longer than the interval between periods of firing; in this way, firing in one period could activate a suppressive mechanism that would affect the subsequent period.

This process matches the eye size with the overall size of the an

This process matches the eye size with the overall size of the animal. Damage to cells in the peripheral retina causes an increase in the proliferation of the progenitor cells in the CMZ and replacement

of the cells that were destroyed by the insult. However, the new cells regenerated by the CMZ do not migrate to central regions of retina and only repair the peripheral damage. Nevertheless, the fact that the retina in fish and amphibians grows throughout their life may require that developmental mechanisms be preserved and provide a partial explanation for their regenerative potential. Because of its ability to regenerate and due to the excellent molecular tools developed in zebrafish, recent studies have begun to identify the molecular requirements for AZD9291 clinical trial regeneration in this species. Neural progenitor genes are upregulated in Müller glia after damage consistent with their shift to the phenotype of a retinal progenitor, while some Müller glial-specific genes are downregulated as the regenerative process proceeds. Although it is not yet known whether the Müller glia are fully reprogrammed to retinal progenitors in fish, several developmentally

important genes have been shown to be necessary for successful regeneration; Dasatinib molecular weight for example, knockdown of the proneural bHLH transcription factor Ascl1a blocks regeneration (Fausett et al., 2008), as does knockdown of proliferating cell nuclear antigen (PCNA) (Thummel et al., 2008). Signaling factors such as Midkine-a and -b, galectin,

and ciliary neurotrophic factor (CNTF) are upregulated after injury and potentially important in the proliferation of the Müller cells that underlies regeneration (Calinescu et al., 2009 and Kassen et al., 2009). Müller glia of posthatch chicks also respond to neurotoxin damage to the retina by re-entering the mitotic cell cycle (Fischer and Reh, 2001). Unlike the fish, however, the Müller glia in the posthatch chick progress through one or at most two cell cycles but do not undergo multiple rounds of cell division. Attempts to stimulate the proliferation with injections of growth factors can prolong this process somewhat and possibly recruit additional Müller the glia into the cell cycle. In addition to a tempered proliferative response by the Müller glia, posthatch chicks show a limited amount of neuronal regeneration. Damage to the retina causes some of the proliferating Müller cells to express most of the progenitor genes that are upregulated in fish Müller glia after damage (Fischer et al., 2002, Fischer and Reh, 2001, Fischer and Reh, 2003 and Hayes et al., 2007). When the progeny of the proliferating Müller glia are tracked over the weeks after damage, BrdU+ cells are found that express markers of amacrine cells (calretinin+, HuC/D+), bipolar cells (Islet1), and occasional ganglion cells (Brn3; neurofilament).

In computational studies, progress has been achieved in understan

In computational studies, progress has been achieved in understanding of how sparse codes can

be generated by neural networks. It was shown that the recurrent network of inhibitory neurons can represent its inputs by sparse codes (Rozell et al., 2008). Understanding of these behaviors has become possible due to the approach based on Lyapunov function (Seung et al., 1998). While these models (Rozell et al., 2008) may provide neuronal mechanisms for sparse codes, they rely on the assumption that feedforward and feedback synaptic weights should satisfy a specific relationship. It is not clear how this condition is implemented biologically. While MCs form the representation of odorants, their number is significantly smaller buy Venetoclax than the number of local inhibitory interneurons, granule cells (GCs), which play an important role in the network interactions. These cells are thought to implement lateral www.selleckchem.com/products/Rapamycin.html inhibition

between MCs through a mechanism based on dendrodendritic reciprocal synapses (Figure 1) (Shepherd et al., 2004). Such interactions facilitate discrimination between similar stimuli and mediate competition between coactive neurons (Arevian et al., 2008). In agreement with this idea, facilitating inhibition between MCs and GCs improves performance in complex but not in simple discrimination tasks (Abraham et al., 2010). In this paper, we study the mathematical model of olfactory bulb. Using this model, we address a series of questions about the responses of MCs to odorants. How can sparse combinatorial code emerge as a result of network activity in the olfactory bulb? That is, how can MCs disregard the inputs from receptor neurons? How can transient (i.e., temporally sparse) activity be generated all by the same network? What is the role of network architecture of the olfactory bulb based on dendrodendritic synapses? How can olfactory code be state dependent, and is there a way to control the responses of MCs in a task-dependent manner? To answer these questions, we propose a novel role for olfactory bulb GCs. We show that GCs can form representations of olfactory

stimuli in the inhibitory inputs that they return to the MCs. MCs transmit to the olfactory cortex the errors of these representations. An exact balance between excitation from receptor neurons and inhibition from the GCs eliminates odorant responses for some MCs; however, other MCs retain the ability to respond to odors due to the incompleteness of the GCs’ representations. This function is facilitated by the network architecture based on dendrodendritic reciprocal synapses between the MCs and the GCs. In this architecture, both feedforward and recurrent connections for the GCs are mediated by the same synapses, thus making biologically plausible the specific relationship between feedforward and feedback synaptic weights necessary for the existence of sparse coding in the current mathematical models (Rozell et al., 2008).

After NMDAR activation, BAPTA blocked changes in rectification, i

After NMDAR activation, BAPTA blocked changes in rectification, implicating the involvement of elevated Ca2+ and supporting a role for NMDARs in this process (n = 8; RI, 0.54 ± 0.06 to 0.57 ± 0.07; p = 0.39). Our hypothesis is that the NMDAR-induced changes in rectification we observe are due to a loss of CI-AMPARs with a possible replacement by CP-AMPARs. There are several different mechanisms by which the loss of CI-AMPARs could occur. One is RNA Synthesis inhibitor through lateral diffusion of AMPARs from the synaptic to the extrasynaptic membrane (Borgdorff and Choquet, 2002). However, the best-characterized mechanism

of AMPAR removal is dynamin-dependent endocytosis, triggered by an NMDAR-induced rise in postsynaptic Ca2+ (Carroll et al., 2001). We tested whether the CI-AMPARs are internalized due to dynamin activity by dialyzing RGCs with 10 mM INCB024360 dynamin-inhibitory peptide (DIP), which blocks endocytosis of AMPARs by interfering with the binding of amphiphysin with dynamin (Lüscher et al., 1999). In RGCs, DIP causes a run up of the extrasynaptic, not synaptic,

AMPAR-mediated response due to unbalanced insertion of AMPARs undergoing rapid cycling (Xia et al., 2007). To ensure that this separate effect of DIP would not confound our results, we first recorded a 10 min baseline of light responses during DIP dialysis before recording the control I-V. The light responses of all ten cells remained stable during this period (3.2% ± 1.3% change over Bumetanide 10 min; data not shown), indicating that synaptic AMPARs under our recording condition are stable and that DIP does not affect the initial AMPAR ratio. While the mean baseline RI was higher in DIP-loaded cells than that of the control cells (Figure 4F; RI = 0.74), this effect was not significant (p = 0.15, t test) and probably reflects the variability of RIs as seen in Figure 1D. We find that inclusion of DIP in the pipette solution consistently

blocked the induction of synaptic plasticity with NMDA. The average rectification was 0.74 ± 0.04 before and 0.73 ± 0.07 after application of NMDA (Figure 4; n = 10, p = 0.64). Although, on average, there was no change in RI, in three out of ten cells, there was an increase in response amplitude at −60mV and no change in amplitude at +40mV. This result suggests that new CP-AMPARs were inserted into the membrane, presumably through persistent exocytosis or membrane diffusion, and supports the hypothesis that NMDAR activation induces an exchange of CI-AMPARs for CP-AMPARs. Our findings suggest that direct pharmacological activation of NMDARs on ON and ON-OFF RGCs drives AMPAR plasticity, but they do not establish whether endogenous transmitter release from presynaptic ON bipolar cells can similarly drive NMDAR-dependent plasticity.

, 2000), though subsequent aspects of structural development at s

, 2000), though subsequent aspects of structural development at some synapses are perturbed (Kummer et al., 2006 and Witzemann et al., 2013). Similarly, we have found that depletion of both evoked and miniature NT disrupts Drosophila synaptic terminal development, particularly of the size of individual synaptic boutons. Surprisingly, however, we found that the specific abolishment of evoked NT using two different transgenic toxins had no effect on synaptic morphology. In contrast, synaptic development was disrupted when miniature NT was specifically depleted by manipulation of postsynaptic glutamate receptors. These phenotypes could be rescued by wild-type receptors, including mammalian

glutamate receptors, but were unaltered by manipulating evoked NT. Oppositely, Doxorubicin we found that increasing miniature NT is sufficient to induce synaptic terminal overgrowth. Using live imaging, we observed that enlargement of synaptic boutons is bidirectionally responsive to changes Anti-diabetic Compound Library high throughput in miniature NT, and we found that this process was coupled with the ultrastructural maturation of synaptic active zones. We determined that miniature NT acts locally at synaptic terminals to regulate bouton maturation via a Trio GEF and Rac1 GTPase molecular signaling pathway. Our data therefore

reveal a unique and specific requirement for miniature events in the development of synaptic terminals that is not shared with and cannot be compensated by evoked NT. These results indicate that miniature neurotransmission, often dismissed as superfluous “noise” from evoked release, has essential and independent functions in vivo in the nervous system. Our data

reveal a surprisingly distinct requirement for miniature NT for normal synaptic development. Like many chemical synapses, the majority of neurotransmitter released at Drosophila NMJ terminals is via evoked NT. Not only is the amplitude of eEPSPs approximately 50-fold larger than mEPSPs old at this terminal, but also evoked release occurs during endogenous activity as frequent rhythmic bursts ( Kurdyak et al., 1994). Despite this, when evoked NT was completely abolished at these terminals, we observed no defects in morphological development, consistent with other studies ( Dickman et al., 2006). Dissection of miniature NT from evoked release was made possible by exploiting synaptic homeostasis ( Davis, 2013 and Petersen et al., 1997), which we show occurs throughout the development of this terminal when postsynaptic glutamate receptors (iGluRs) are inhibited. Replacement of endogenous iGluRs by mutant subunits resulted in conditions where evoked NT was similar to controls, due to a relative increase in the number of synaptic vesicles released per action potential, but miniature NT was dramatically decreased. In these mutants, where miniature NT is depleted far more severely than in previous reports (e.g., dGluRIIA mutants; Petersen et al., 1997; data not shown), synaptic maturation was specifically perturbed.

Similar to those in HDL2 patients, the RNA foci were rarely coloc

Similar to those in HDL2 patients, the RNA foci were rarely colocalized with NIs and were colocalized with Mbnl1 (Figure S2B). Selleckchem GSK1349572 Thus, we concluded that BAC-HDL2 mice also recapitulate the phenotype of CUG RNA foci, another

molecular pathological marker for HDL2. One intriguing finding in HDL2 neuropathology is the immunoreactivity of NIs with 1C2, a monoclonal antibody that has relatively high specificity to all expanded neuropathogenic polyQ proteins (Trottier et al., 1995), but can also recognize some normal long polyQ proteins such as TBP as well as some other amino acid stretches such as polyleucine (Dorsman et al., 2002). Because of this latter possibility, the precise molecular nature of the 1C2 immunoreactivity within NIs in HDL2 remains to be clarified. We next asked whether the NIs in BAC-HDL2 mice, like those in HDL2 patients, could be immunostained with 1C2. By using a sensitive antigen retrieval technique (Osmand et al., 2006) we were able to detect 1C2-immunoreactive NIs in 12-month-old BAC-HLD2

brains that are unlike the faint diffuse nuclear staining found in the Volasertib mw wild-type controls (Figure S3A). Such 1C2 (+) NIs were not detected at 1 month old, but could be detected at 3 months old and became progressively enlarged at 6 and 12 months old (Figures S3A and S3C). Finally, double immunofluorescent staining revealed that 1C2-immunoreactive NIs colocalized with ubiquitin-positive NIs (Figure S3B), suggesting that the composition of NIs in BAC-HDL2 mice is quite similar to those described in HDL2 patients. To provide further evidence that BAC-HDL2 NIs contain an expanded polyQ protein, we used another monoclonal antibody, 3B5H10, which has been shown to be specific to the expanded crotamiton polyQ epitope in all known polyQ disorders (Brooks et al., 2004). Immunostaining with 3B5H10 after antigen retrieval revealed that NIs in 12-month-old BAC-HDL2 cortices and striatum were prominently stained with this expanded polyQ-specific antibody (Figure 3). No such 3B5H10 (+) NIs were detected in the brains of wild-type control littermates at 12 months old (Figure 3). Importantly, the distribution of

3B5H10-immunoreactive NIs in BAC-HDL2 brains is strikingly similar to that of patients, with prominent levels of NIs in the cortex (the upper cortical layers more than the deep cortical layers), hippocampus, and amygdala, decreased abundance in the striatum, and very few if any NIs detected in the cerebellum, thalamus, and brain stem (Figure 4 and data not shown). Taken together, our neuropathological studies with both 1C2 and 3B5H10 antibodies demonstrated that the NIs found in BAC-HDL2 brains recapitulate the patterns seen in HDL2 patients. Furthermore, an expanded polyQ protein is probably a component of such NIs. Because pathogenesis of HDL2 has been linked to the expansion of CTG/CAG repeats at the human JPH3 locus ( Holmes et al.

From these parameters, we estimate that the time required to move

From these parameters, we estimate that the time required to move the probe is 1.3 ms. Thus, the latency for channel activation is 2.1 ms or less. This latency is longer than the shortest latencies measured for other C. elegans neurons ( O’Hagan et al., 2005 and Kang

et al., 2010), but, because the fastest known second messenger-based sensory transduction pathway has a latency of 20 ms ( Hardie, 2001), we propose that this latency is brief enough to suggest that force acts directly on the MeT channels that carry MRCs in ASH. Sinusoidal oscillations were detected in many of our MRC recordings Compound Library suggesting that channel activation is able to follow the rapid, resonant movements of the probe (Figure 1B). To determine the frequency of MRC oscillations, we fit the total MRC with an alpha function and subtracted this fit from the average current to isolate the sinusoidal variations in current (Figure 1B). In five recordings with high-quality oscillations, the MRC oscillation frequency had an average value of 130 ± 6 Hz (mean ± SEM, n = 5). Thus, channels carrying MRCs in the ASH neurons can follow rapid variations in applied mechanical loads. Mechanoreceptor currents, if mediated by a DEG/ENaC channel complex, should be carried by Na+ ions and blocked by amiloride. Conversely, if MRCs were carried by a TRPV channel complex, they should be permeable to both Na+

and K+ and resistant to amiloride. Wild-type MRCs were reversibly

Selleck BIBW2992 blocked by amiloride (Figures 2A and 2B). The fraction of peak current blocked by 300 μM amiloride was 0.77 ± 0.06 (n = 4) and 0.75 ± 0.10 (n = 3) at −90 and −60 mV, respectively. This same level of MRC block was achieved in the gentle touch receptor neuron PLM that expresses the DEG/ENaC channel subunits MEC-4 and MEC-10 with 200 μM amiloride (O’Hagan et al., 2005). MRCs in ASH may be carried by DEG/ENaC channels that are more resistant to amiloride than MEC-4 and MEC-10 or ASH may express a distinct population of channels that is insensitive to amiloride. Below, we provide evidence that MRCs are carried by two classes of ion channels. The ASH neurons terminate in a single cilium before that extends into the external environment through an opening in the amphid (Perkins et al., 1986). If the MeT channels localize to this cilium, then exogenous amiloride should inhibit behavioral responses to nose touch. Consistent with this prediction, animals exposed to amiloride for more than 30 min showed a modest but statistically significant decrease in sensitivity to nose touch (Figure 2C). Such a minor effect on nose touch sensitivity is the expected result for two reasons. First, 300 μM amiloride does not completely block MRCs (Figures 2A and 2B). Second, ASH is not the only mechanoreceptor neuron responsible for sensitivity to nose touch (Kaplan and Horvitz, 1993), but it is the only one exposed to the external environment.

This is when monkeys, based on learning a few S-R associations, c

This is when monkeys, based on learning a few S-R associations, could first start to predict the saccade that would DAPT mw lead to reward. Rise time in STR averaged 130.7 ± 12.9 ms (SEM) across trials of the S-R

association phase. This is in contrast to PFC, where average rise time was significantly later, at 822.1 ± 128.2 ms (p < 5 × 10−4, Figure 3B). Likewise, during the early-trial epoch (exemplar display and the first half of the delay), information about the forthcoming saccade was significantly higher in STR (1.90 ± 0.04) than PFC (1.0 ± 0.04, p < 10−4, Figure 3C, left). In contrast, late in the trial (second half of the delay and during saccade execution), saccade information

was stronger in PFC (2.44 ± 0.05) than STR (0.83 ± 0.05, p < 10−4, Figure 3C, right). These results indicate that STR played a more leading role than PFC when performance relied on specific S-R associations. A comparison of correct and error trials during the S-R phase is shown in Figure 4. In both cases, monkeys execute a right or left saccade. If activity reflects a motor signal per se, information should be equal on both. Yet, early-trial information in STR was greatly reduced on error versus correct trials (0.02 ± 0.04, p < 10−4, Figures 4A and 4B). It was lower when correct and error trials were pooled together and classified according to exemplar (1.38 ± 0.04, p <

10−4, Figure 4C), Lapatinib or saccade (0.70 ± 0.03, p < 10−4, Figure 4D). Fossariinae There was also a decrease in PFC saccade information late in error trials (error trials alone: 0.85 ± 0.04, p < 10−4; correct and error trials by exemplar: 0.70 ± 0.05, p < 10−4; correct and error trials by saccade: 1.68 ± 0.06, p < 10−4). The lower information on error trials indicate that the STR and PFC are not reflecting a saccade motor plan per se (including “guesses”), but rather are involved in learning the correct saccade. The saccadic motor plan might have been generated and maintained elsewhere. During the category acquisition phase, monkeys were confronted with increasingly larger numbers of novel exemplars (Figure 1C) and had to move beyond simple S-R association and associate the right and left saccades with each category rather than individual exemplars. Performance was maintained at a high level and improved, even though with each block an increasing proportion of novel exemplars was introduced (Figure 3A, middle row). During this phase, strong early-trial, saccade-predicting activity in PFC first appeared. This was reflected in the sharp reduction in rise time (Figure 3B) and increase in saccade-direction information in the early-trial PFC activity, relative to S-R association (p < 0.005 for rise time and p < 10−4 for information magnitude, Figure 3C).